With every article and podcast episode, we provide comprehensive study materials: References, Executive Summary, Briefing Document, Quiz, Essay Questions, Glossary, Timeline, Cast, FAQ, Table of Contents, Index, Polls, 3k Image, and Fact Check.
**** Insert: SOUNDBITE
What if I told you that nearly a quarter of your thoughts happen without words, images, or any identifiable symbols? And what if I told you that some people experience up to 80% of their thinking this way—while others never do?
We've been sold a lie about how our minds work.
For generations, we've been told that thinking is fundamentally verbal—that our inner monologue represents the core of our cognitive life. This assumption runs so deep in our understanding of consciousness that most of us have never questioned it. When asked to describe our thoughts, we instinctively search for words, images, or symbols that might capture them.
But what if our conscious experience extends far beyond language?
The Secret World of Unsymbolized Thinking
The research highlighted in the recent Heliox podcast reveals a fascinating phenomenon called "unsymbolized thinking" (UT)—the experience of having specific, definite thoughts without any awareness of those thoughts being conveyed through words, images, or symbols.
This isn't vague intuition or gut feeling. These are concrete, specific thoughts with clear meaning that appear fully formed in consciousness—but without the symbolic wrapping we assume all thoughts must have.
Even more astonishing is that many people who experience this phenomenon only recognize it when specifically prompted to analyze their thought processes. Using a research method called "descriptive experience sampling" (DES), researchers gave participants random beepers that would signal them to freeze and capture whatever was happening in their minds at that exact moment.
The results? Unsymbolized thinking appeared in 22% of sampled experiences. But here's where things get truly weird: the variation between individuals was enormous. Some people reported no unsymbolized thinking at all, while others experienced it up to 80% of the time.
That's right—some people are walking around with the vast majority of their thoughts occurring without words or images.
The Aborted Speech Hypothesis
So what exactly is happening here? Vicente and Martinez-Manrique propose that unsymbolized thinking represents "aborted inner speech"—thoughts that start down the path toward verbal expression but get cut off before reaching the phonological stage.
When we normally engage in inner speech, our brain simulates what it would be like to actually speak those words out loud. It sends signals to the muscles involved in speech production (vocal cords, tongue, lips) and creates a prediction of what it would feel like to say those words. This predicted feeling is what we experience as inner speech.
But what if this process gets interrupted earlier? What if all that becomes conscious is a prediction about the meaning itself, skipping the sounds completely?
The meaning would still be structured by language, drawing on our linguistic knowledge and semantic representations. The linguistic framework would still be there shaping the thought, but the actual sound of the words would never form.
This explains why unsymbolized thinking feels propositional and why we can often put it into words afterward. The meaning is already there, ready to be expressed, but the vocalization never quite happens.
The Spectrum of Linguistic Experience
What emerges from this research is a fascinating spectrum of linguistic experience:
At one end, we have full-blown inner speech with words, rhythm, and everything else
In the middle, we have "unworded speech" with rhythm and meaning but no words
At the far end, we have unsymbolized thinking with just the bare meanings
Unworded speech is particularly interesting—it's described as experiencing all the vocal characteristics like rhythm, inflection, and pace, along with direct awareness of meaning, but without actual words. It's like having a conversation in your head without the words.
This spectrum suggests that pure meaning might be more fundamental than we realized, with verbal expression being an additional layer rather than the foundation of thought.
Beyond Language: Buddhist Perspectives
This scientific discovery resonates powerfully with certain Buddhist teachings, especially the emphasis on mindfulness and direct experience beyond the limitations of thought and language.
Buddhist teachings often describe the mind as a mirror that can reflect reality perfectly, but our constant stream of thoughts and language clouds that mirror. Practices like meditation aim to cultivate awareness beyond symbolic layers—training minds to quiet down and experience reality directly.
The Buddhist concept of "emptiness" (sunyata) suggests that all phenomena, including thoughts and concepts, lack inherent fixed self-existence—they're not as solid as we believe them to be. True understanding arises when we can see beyond labels and categories.
The early Buddhist concept of "nama" refers to language and naming, seen as a powerful force shaping our experience of the world. One Buddhist text refers to nama as a "universal oppressor," highlighting how language can constrain our understanding of reality.
Even when we think we're escaping language, its structures may still influence us at deep, unconscious levels.
What This Means For You
The discovery of unsymbolized thinking raises profound questions about consciousness, language, and the nature of thought itself:
How much of your thinking happens beyond words? The next time you have a sudden realization or clear idea that pops into your head, pay attention to how it feels. Is it accompanied by words or images, or is it something more immediate and direct?
Are you missing parts of your own mental life? If you've never noticed unsymbolized thinking, you might be overlooking a significant portion of your cognitive experience.
Could language be limiting your understanding? If thinking can happen without symbols, could our reliance on language actually constrain certain types of understanding?
What might be possible if we cultivated awareness of these wordless thoughts? Could developing greater sensitivity to unsymbolized thinking enhance creativity, insight, or problem-solving?
The exploration of unsymbolized thinking reminds us that consciousness remains one of the greatest frontiers in science—and that our subjective experience contains depths we've barely begun to chart.
Next time you "just know" something without being able to explain how you know it, remember: you might be experiencing the pure meaning that exists before words, the foundation of thought itself.
And that realization might change how you listen—not just to others, but to the quiet spaces between your own thoughts where meaning emerges before language gives it shape.
Link References
Episode Links
Youtube
BuzzSprout
Substack
3D Force Model
Other Links to Heliox Podcast
YouTube
Substack
Podcast Providers
Spotify
Apple Podcasts
Patreon
FaceBook Group
STUDY MATERIALS
Briefing Document
1. Definition and Characteristics of Unsymbolized Thought:
The concept of unsymbolized thought, as primarily explored by researchers like Russell Hurlburt using the DES method, refers to the experience of "thinking a particular, definite thought without the awareness of that thought’s being conveyed in words, images, or any other symbols" (Heavey and Hurlburt, 2008: 802). Key characteristics identified through DES include:
Distinct Phenomenon: UT is considered a unique category of inner experience, not reducible to other forms like inner speech or imagery.
Experiential: It is a specific way of experiencing mental content.
Thought-like: It is experienced as thinking, distinct from feelings, intentions, or bodily sensations.
Explicit Content: The subject matter of the thought ("the about what") is clearly apprehended.
Differentiated Content: The specifics of the thought ("the what about it") are not vague but clearly articulated. Together, explicitness and differentiation suggest a clearly articulated sense.
Directly in Experience: The content is immediately present in consciousness.
Atemporal Presentation: UT typically appears "all at once; there is no rhythm or cadence; no unfolding or sequentiality."
Lack of Symbols: It does not involve the conscious experience of words, images, or other symbols.
2. Propositional Nature and Effability of UT:
Vicente and Martínez-Manrique (20XX) argue that the characteristics of UT point towards two main features:
Propositional Phenomenon: Based on the explicitness and differentiated nature of UT, they contend that it has a propositional structure, analogous to the semantic content of sentences, with subject and predicate-like components. As Hurlburt and Akhter (2008: 1367) note, "Just as a complete sentence contains a subject (the about what) and a predicate (the what about it), the typical unsymbolized thought can be said to have those characteristics." This suggests that UT possesses underlying conceptual and syntactic features.
Effability: Despite the lack of conscious symbols, UTs are generally effable, meaning they can be given a reasonably straightforward linguistic rendition. Subjects can typically verbalize the content of their UT, even if they might consider several similar phrasings.
3. Unsymbolized Thought as Aborted Inner Speech:
Vicente and Martínez-Manrique propose that the best explanation for the properties of UT is that it is continuous with inner speech. Specifically, they hypothesize that "unsymbolized thinking is continuous with the activity of inner speech, in particular, it is a form of inner speech where the speech action is aborted even before the intention to talk is implemented by motor commands."
Their argument rests on the idea that the process of inner speech involves a cascade of intentions, from the initial desire to express a thought to the generation of motor commands. Predictions about the sensory feedback of the intended speech are made at different levels. They suggest that UT arises when this process is aborted early, before reaching the phonological level. In such cases, "only one kind of prediction will be issued, namely, a prediction about the meaning of the message, which will be experienced as such a meaning –and only a meaning." This explains why UT feels like a definite thought that can be easily verbalized, as it draws upon underlying semantic and syntactic structures prepared for linguistic expression.
4. Agency and Ownership in Unsymbolized Thinking:
The proposed link to inner speech also offers an explanation for the sense of agency associated with thinking. Drawing on comparator models of action control, Vicente and Martínez-Manrique argue that the feeling of agency arises when predictions and incoming signals match. In the context of UT, the "prediction about the meaning" is triggered by an "intention to speak which is immediately aborted," leading to a feeling of authorship over the thought.
5. Comparison with Unworded Speech:
The sources also touch upon "unworded speech," described as the "experience of speaking in your own inner voice except that you have no experience of words at all" (Hurlburt and Heavey 2006, 211). While seemingly similar to UT, unworded speech retains a sense of "vocal characteristics" like rate and rhythm, which are absent in the atemporal experience of UT. Vicente and Martínez-Manrique suggest that both phenomena can be seen as points on a continuum of aborted inner speech, with UT representing an even earlier abortion where the temporal and phonetic aspects associated with the "emission" of speech are also lacking, leaving primarily the structured meaning.
6. Buddhist Perspective and Unsymbolized Experience:
The "Pasted Text" introduces the Buddhist perspective, which "emphasizes mindfulness and direct experience," aligning with the idea of transcending the limitations of thought and language for true understanding. The text posits that "thought arises first then language" and that "being truely present precludes consious symbolization of any sort."
Key Buddhist concepts highlighted:
Mind as a Mirror: The mind reflects reality, but thoughts and emotions can obscure this reflection.
Mindfulness: Practices aim to cultivate direct awareness of the present moment, beyond the constraints of thought and language.
Emptiness (Sunyata): All phenomena, including thoughts and concepts, lack inherent self-existence. Recognizing this emptiness leads to deeper understanding.
The concept of Nāma in early Buddhism is presented as "the pivot around which the entire early Buddhist conception of language revolves." In SN 1.61, Nāma is identified as "What oppresses everything? What has nothing greater than itself? What is the one thing that holds everything under its sway?" The answer to all these questions is Nāma, indicating that early Buddhism viewed language as a powerful and potentially oppressive force. This perspective resonates with the idea that true presence and understanding might lie beyond symbolic representation.
7. Implications for Cognitive Phenomenology:
Vicente and Martínez-Manrique discuss the implications of UT for the debate on cognitive phenomenology:
While UT suggests a general "what it is like" to think, it doesn't necessarily imply a distinct phenomenology for every specific thought content (thinking that p vs. thinking that q). The initial surprise of experiencing UT highlights the novelty of thinking without symbols, rather than the novelty of each specific unsymbolized thought.
The dependence of UT's semantic and syntactic features on language production mechanisms suggests that even unsymbolized thought is not necessarily "pure" or linguistically uncontaminated. The experience of UT is linked to the same underlying structures that give rise to the phenomenology of linguistic experience.
The feeling of agency in UT can be explained through its connection to aborted intentions to speak, supporting the idea that conscious thinking is an active process.
8. Conclusion:
The sources collectively present a compelling picture of unsymbolized thought as a genuine and significant aspect of inner experience. Research utilizing the DES method provides detailed phenomenological descriptions, while theoretical accounts propose that UT is best understood as an aborted form of inner speech, retaining its propositional structure and connection to language production mechanisms. The Buddhist perspective offers a broader philosophical context that values direct, unsymbolized experience as a path to deeper understanding. Further research is needed to fully elucidate the nature and implications of UT for our understanding of consciousness, thought, and the relationship between language and cognition.
Quiz & Answer Key
Answer the following questions in 2-3 sentences each.
According to the provided texts, what is unsymbolized thought?
How does the Buddhist perspective relate to the concept of transcending thought and language?
What is the Descriptive Experience Sampling (DES) method, and what has it revealed about unsymbolized thought?
What are some of the key characteristics of unsymbolized thought as identified by Hurlburt and Akhter?
What is the authors' minimal characterization of unsymbolized thought in terms of its nature?
How do the authors argue that unsymbolized thinking might be related to inner speech?
What is the concept of "unworded speech" as described in the texts, and how does it differ from unsymbolized thought?
How do the authors connect the feeling of agency to their explanation of unsymbolized thought?
What do the authors suggest about the relationship between unsymbolized thought and the categories of natural language?
What is the significance of unsymbolized thought for the cognitive phenomenology debate, according to the authors?
Quiz Answer Key
Unsymbolized thought is described as the experience of having a specific, definite thought without being aware of it being conveyed in words, images, or any other symbols. Subjects reporting this phenomenon often initially struggle to characterize it, distinguishing it from inner speech due to the lack of symbolic features.
The Buddhist perspective aligns with the idea that true understanding and liberation arise from going beyond the limitations of thought and language. Practices like mindfulness aim to cultivate direct awareness of the present moment, reflecting a reality unfiltered by symbolic processing.
The Descriptive Experience Sampling (DES) method, developed by Hurlburt and collaborators, involves subjects carrying a beeper that randomly prompts them to record their inner experience at that precise moment. This method has revealed that unsymbolized thinking is a distinct and relatively common phenomenon, often unexpected by those who experience it.
Key characteristics of unsymbolized thought include being a distinct phenomenon, a way of experiencing (not just feeling), having explicit and differentiated content that is directly in experience, typically presenting all at once without sequentiality, and lacking the experience of words or other symbols.
The authors minimally characterize unsymbolized thought as a propositional phenomenon, meaning it has semantic and syntactic features analogous to the contents of utterances. They also argue that it is effable, meaning it can be rendered linguistically even though it doesn't appear in words initially.
The authors hypothesize that unsymbolized thinking is continuous with inner speech, suggesting it is a form of inner speech where the speech action is aborted before the intention to talk is fully implemented by motor commands. They argue this best explains the semantic and syntactic similarities and the sense of agency.
Unworded speech is described as the experience of speaking in one's inner voice without the experience of words, although there might be an awareness of vocal characteristics like rate and inflection, as well as the meaning. It differs from unsymbolized thought, which lacks the temporal and vocal characteristics associated with the sense of speaking.
The authors suggest that the sense of agency associated with unsymbolized thought can be explained by their model of it as aborted inner speech. The intention to speak and the involvement of the motor control system (efference copies, predictions) contribute to the feeling of authorship, even if the speech act is never fully realized.
The authors argue that the effability and familiarity of unsymbolized thought suggest that it draws on the syntax and semantics of one's own natural language. They contend that the conceptual structure of unsymbolized thought appears linguistically prepackaged, arranged according to the rules of the subject's language.
According to the authors, while unsymbolized thought shows that thinking has a generic phenomenology, it doesn't clearly demonstrate that each specific thought content has a distinct phenomenology. They also argue that its reliance on underlying linguistic mechanisms suggests that even unsymbolized thought is not necessarily "pure" or linguistically uncontaminated.
Essay Questions
Discuss the implications of the existence of unsymbolized thought for our understanding of the relationship between thought and language. Consider arguments from both the provided texts and potential counterarguments.
Analyze the authors' argument for the continuity between unsymbolized thought and inner speech. What are the strengths and weaknesses of their explanation, and are there alternative explanations that could account for the phenomenon?
Compare and contrast the experiential phenomena of unsymbolized thought and unworded speech as described in the texts. How do these distinctions challenge or support theories of inner experience and consciousness?
Explore the connection between the sense of agency and the experience of unsymbolized thought, according to the authors. How does their "aborted inner speech" hypothesis explain this feeling of authorship, and are there other perspectives on the agency of thought?
Critically evaluate the claim that unsymbolized thought provides evidence for or against the idea of "pure" or linguistically uncontaminated thought. Consider the authors' arguments about its semantic and syntactic characteristics in your analysis.
Glossary of Key Terms
Unsymbolized Thought (UT): The experience of having a definite thought without being aware of it being conveyed through words, images, or other symbols.
Mindfulness: A Buddhist practice involving focused awareness of the present moment without judgment, often aimed at gaining deeper insight into the nature of reality.
Direct Experience: Knowledge or understanding gained through immediate sensory or introspective awareness, without the mediation of thought or language.
Emptiness (Sunyata): A central concept in Buddhism indicating that all phenomena, including thoughts and concepts, lack inherent self-existence.
Nāma: An early Buddhist concept often understood in relation to "name" or "form" (rūpa), playing a crucial role in the Buddhist understanding of language and its relationship to consciousness.
Rūpa: An early Buddhist concept referring to "form" or physical phenomena, often paired with nāma.
Descriptive Experience Sampling (DES): A research method developed by Russell Hurlburt to capture detailed descriptions of inner experiences as they occur in real-time.
Propositional State: A mental state that has a specific content that can be expressed as a proposition (a statement that can be true or false), often involving conceptual and syntactic structure.
Inner Speech: The internal monologue or self-talk that individuals experience.
Aborted Inner Speech: The hypothesis that unsymbolized thought arises from an intention to engage in inner speech that is interrupted before the full production of words or symbols.
Cognitive Phenomenology: The study of the "what it's like" aspect of cognitive states, such as thinking and understanding.
Effable: Capable of being expressed or described in words.
Mentalese (Language of Thought - LOT): A hypothetical internal representational system in the mind that is thought to underlie and enable thought.
Motor Imagery: The mental rehearsal or simulation of a motor act without overt physical movement.
Efference Copy (Corollary Discharge): A neural signal that is a copy of a motor command sent to other brain areas, used for predicting the sensory consequences of an action and distinguishing self-generated actions from external events.
Forward Models: Internal models in the brain that predict the sensory consequences of motor commands.
Unworded Speech (Unworded Inner Speech): The experience of speaking in one's inner voice without the awareness of specific words.
Agency: The feeling of being in control of one's own actions and thoughts, the sense of authorship.
Whorfian Hypothesis (Linguistic Relativity): The idea that the structure of a language influences the way its speakers conceptualize and perceive the world.
Timeline of Main Events
Early Buddhist Period (as indicated by SN 1.61):
Concept of Nāma Emerges: Early Buddhist thought identifies "nāma" (name, concept, linguistic designation) as a central element in the understanding of language and cognition.
Nāma as a Universal Oppressor: The SN 1.61 text portrays nāma as something that "oppresses everything," has "nothing greater than itself," and holds "everything under its sway," highlighting the powerful influence attributed to language/conceptualization.
Nāma and Rūpa Dyad: Nāma is understood as part of a fundamental cognitive dyad with "rūpa" (form, physical aspect), suggesting an early recognition of the interplay between conceptual and physical realities.
Relationship with Consciousness: Early Buddhist thought recognizes a unique relationship between nāma and consciousness.
Modern Research on Unsymbolized Thinking:
Russell Hurlburt's Research: Researchers like Russell Hurlburt begin exploring the phenomenon of "unsymbolized thinking" (UT) as a distinct form of inner experience.
Development of Descriptive Experience Sampling (DES): Hurlburt and collaborators develop the DES method to collect data about people's inner experiences in close proximity to their occurrence. This method involves participants carrying a beeper and recording their experience when it sounds.
Findings of UT through DES: Studies using DES reveal that some individuals report experiencing thoughts that do not involve words, images, or any other symbols. The frequency of UT varies significantly between individuals.
Initial Subject Reactions to UT: Subjects who experience UT often find it unexpected and initially struggle to categorize it, sometimes attempting to fit it into the framework of inner speech before realizing its lack of symbolic features.
Hurlburt et al.'s Characterization of UT: Hurlburt and Akhter (2008) outline key characteristics of UT, including it being a distinct phenomenon, a way of experiencing, a form of thinking (not feeling or other sensations), having explicit and differentiated content directly in experience, typically presenting all at once without sequentiality, and lacking words or other symbols.
Distinction Between Inner Speaking and Inner Hearing: Based on DES, Hurlburt et al. (2013) differentiate between inner speaking (feeling like you are talking to yourself) and inner hearing (feeling like you are listening to an inner voice).
Neurological Correlates of Inner Speech: fMRI studies (Kühn et al., 2014; Hurlburt et al., 2016) begin to find different neurological bases for inner speaking and inner hearing, providing some support for the distinctions made through DES.
Vicente and Martínez-Manrique's Analysis of UT:
UT as Propositional: Vicente and Martínez-Manrique (2013) analyze UT as appearing to be propositional, with semantic and syntactic features analogous to the contents of utterances.
UT as Effable: They argue that unsymbolized thoughts are generally effable, meaning they can be given a reasonably straightforward linguistic rendition, suggesting a "linguistically prepackaged" conceptual structure.
UT and Inner Speech Continuity Hypothesis: Vicente and Martínez-Manrique propose that UT is continuous with the activity of inner speech, specifically as a form of inner speech where the speech action is aborted before motor commands are fully implemented.
Explanation of Agency and Ownership in UT: Their account suggests that the sense of agency associated with UT stems from the initiation of an intention to speak and the involvement of the motor control system, even if the action is aborted.
Discussion of Unworded Speech: They address the related phenomenon of "unworded speech" (inner speech without the experience of words) reported by Hurlburt et al., suggesting it might represent a different stage of aborted inner speech or "condensed inner speech" with a verbal origin.
Implications for Cognitive Phenomenology: They discuss how UT, while showing a generic phenomenology of thinking, doesn't necessarily demonstrate a unique phenomenology for each specific thought content. They also argue that UT is not necessarily "pure thought" uncontaminated by linguistic categories.
Buddhist Perspective on Mind and Language:
Mind as a Mirror: Buddhist teachings describe the mind as a mirror reflecting reality, obscured by the constant stream of thoughts and emotions.
Mindfulness and Direct Experience: Practices like meditation aim to cultivate direct awareness of the present moment, leading to understanding beyond thought and language.
Concept of Emptiness (Sunyata): Buddhism posits that all phenomena, including thoughts and concepts, are ultimately empty of inherent self-existence, and recognizing this emptiness is key to true understanding.
Alignment with Transcending Thought: The Buddhist emphasis on mindfulness and direct experience aligns with the idea that true understanding and liberation come from transcending the limitations of thought and language, echoing the significance of experiences beyond symbolization explored in the UT research.
Cast of Characters
Russell Hurlburt: A researcher who has extensively explored the phenomenon of inner experience, particularly through the development and application of the Descriptive Experience Sampling (DES) method. He is known for his work on unsymbolized thinking, inner speech, and other aspects of consciousness.
Agustín Vicente: A researcher at Ikerbasque: Basque Foundation for Science and the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU). He is a co-author of the paper analyzing the nature and origin of unsymbolized thinking, proposing its continuity with inner speech.
Fernando Martínez-Manrique: A researcher in the Department of Philosophy I at the University of Granada, Spain. He is the co-author with Agustín Vicente, contributing to the analysis of unsymbolized thinking and the development of their theoretical account.
Dorothy (Subject in Hurlburt and Akhter's report): An individual who, during a DES session, reported an experience of unsymbolized thinking that could be verbalized as something like, "Pick up your feet—it sounds like an old lady," but without experiencing any words or symbols. Her report is used as a key example in the discussion of UT's characteristics and effability.
Evelyn (Subject in Hurlburt and Akhter's report): Another participant in Hurlburt's DES studies who, upon experiencing unsymbolized thought for the first time (wondering if NetZero is cheaper than Cox Cable), reacted with surprise, illustrating the distinct phenomenology of this type of experience.
Peter Carruthers: A philosopher who has offered critiques of the DES method and suggested that reports of unsymbolized thinking might be a result of confabulation (unintentionally false memory).
Helmholtz: A 19th-century scientist who proposed the corollary discharge model of perception, a concept that later influenced theories of motor control and the sense of agency, including its application to inner speech and unsymbolized thinking.
von Holst & Mittelstaedt: Researchers who extended Helmholtz's corollary discharge model to the realm of motor acts, further developing the idea of comparing predicted sensory feedback with actual feedback to understand self-initiated actions.
Sperry: Another researcher who contributed to the development of the corollary discharge model in relation to motor control and the perception of self-agency.
Marc Jeannerod: A researcher known for his work on motor imagery and cognition, whose research suggests that motor imagery can involve predictions based on motor commands that are inhibited, an idea that Vicente and Martínez-Manrique apply to their account of inner speech and UT.
Charles Fernyhough: A researcher who has investigated inner speech and auditory verbal hallucinations, proposing the concept of "condensed inner speech," which is discussed in relation to unworded speech.
Lev Vygotsky: A prominent psychologist whose work is referenced in the context of "thinking in pure meanings," a concept related to condensed inner speech and potentially unworded speech.
FAQ
1. What is unsymbolized thinking (UT) as described by researchers like Hurlburt? Unsymbolized thinking (UT) refers to the experience of having a distinct, definite thought without any awareness of that thought being conveyed through words, images, or other symbols. Individuals who report UT experience it as a thinking, not a feeling or other type of internal event, with a clear and specific content that is directly present in their experience. A key characteristic is that UT typically occurs "all at once" without a sense of unfolding or sequentiality, and crucially, without any accompanying experience of language or other symbolic representation.
2. How does the Buddhist perspective relate to the concept of unsymbolized thought? The Buddhist perspective, with its emphasis on mindfulness and direct experience, aligns with the idea that true understanding and liberation come from transcending the limitations of thought and language. Buddhist teachings often describe the mind as a mirror obscured by a constant stream of thoughts and emotions. Practices like meditation aim to cultivate direct awareness of the present moment, fostering a deeper understanding of reality that exists beyond conscious symbolization. The concept of "emptiness" (sunyata) further suggests that all phenomena, including thoughts and concepts, lack inherent self-existence, implying that true understanding involves recognizing this emptiness beyond linguistic and conceptual frameworks.
3. What is "nāma" in early Buddhist thought, and how does it relate to language and consciousness? In early Buddhist thought, "nāma" (name) is considered a pivotal concept in understanding language. It forms a fundamental cognitive dyad with "rūpa" (form) but also functions as an independent agent with its own characteristics. Notably, nāma holds a unique relationship with consciousness. A significant early Buddhist text (SN 1.61) portrays nāma as the universal oppressor, the one thing that holds everything under its sway, with nothing greater than itself. This highlights that early Buddhism did not view language as trivial but as a powerful force shaping our experience.
4. How do the researchers Vicente and Martínez-Manrique characterize unsymbolized thinking? Vicente and Martínez-Manrique analyze UT as a propositional phenomenon, meaning that even without symbols, UTs are experienced as compositional conceptual states with semantic and syntactic features analogous to the contents of utterances. They argue that UTs are effable, meaning they can be given a relatively straightforward linguistic rendition, even though they don't initially appear in words. Based on these characteristics, they propose that UT is continuous with the activity of inner speech, specifically as a form of inner speech where the speech act is aborted before the intention to speak is fully implemented by motor commands.
5. What evidence supports the existence of unsymbolized thinking, and what are some challenges to this idea? The primary method used to gather evidence for UT is Descriptive Experience Sampling (DES), where subjects are prompted at random times to describe their inner experiences. Reports from DES studies indicate that some individuals do experience thoughts without words or other symbols, and that this phenomenon is often unexpected even for those who experience it. Challenges to the existence of UT include methodological criticisms of DES and the suggestion that reports of UT might be confabulations, where individuals mistakenly believe they are thinking without symbols. However, proponents argue that the consistency of reports and the surprise of subjects experiencing UT for the first time argue against mere confabulation.
6. How do Vicente and Martínez-Manrique explain the origin and nature of unsymbolized thinking? Vicente and Martínez-Manrique propose that UT originates as an aborted act of inner speech. They suggest that the process of inner speech begins with a non-conscious intention to express a thought, which becomes increasingly specific, engaging semantic, syntactic, and phonological representations and eventually motor commands. They hypothesize that UT occurs when this process is aborted before reaching the phonological level. The result is a prediction about the meaning of the intended message, experienced directly as a thought content without accompanying symbolic or perceptual elements.
7. How does the theory of UT as aborted inner speech account for the sense of agency associated with thinking? The sense of agency, the feeling that we are the authors of our thoughts and actions, can be explained through the lens of the corollary discharge model. This model suggests that we feel agency when our predictions about the outcomes of our actions match the incoming sensory signals. Vicente and Martínez-Manrique extend this to the mental domain, arguing that in UT, the prediction about the meaning of a linguistic expression (stemming from an aborted intention to speak) is experienced. We feel authorship because this prediction is triggered by our intention, even if the full speech act is not realized. This contrasts with views that see conscious thoughts as simply occurring without intention or agency.
8. What are the implications of unsymbolized thinking for the debate on cognitive phenomenology? UT suggests that there is a proprietary phenomenology to thinking, distinct from perceiving or saying. However, it doesn't as clearly demonstrate that each specific thought content has a unique phenomenology that differentiates it from other thoughts. The surprise often reported by individuals experiencing UT for the first time indicates a novel kind of experience (thinking without symbols), rather than a novel feeling for each new unsymbolized thought content. Furthermore, the connection of UT to the mechanisms of language production suggests that its semantic-related phenomenological properties are linked to those of linguistic experience, implying that even "pure thought" as experienced in UT might be intertwined with linguistic categories.
Table of Contents with Timestamps
00:00 - Introduction to Heliox
The podcast begins with a brief introduction to Heliox: Where Evidence Meets Empathy, describing its approach to important topics.
00:24 - Starting the Conversation
The hosts begin discussing those moments when clear thoughts appear in our minds without words.
00:45 - Defining Unsymbolized Thinking
Introduction to the concept of unsymbolized thinking - thinking without words, images, or symbols.
01:15 - Research Background
Discussion of "The Nature of Unsymbolized Thinking" book and the research method used.
01:36 - Descriptive Experience Sampling Method
Explanation of the random beeper method used to capture immediate mental experiences.
02:04 - Research Findings on Prevalence
The hosts discuss how unsymbolized thinking appeared in 22% of sampled experiences with huge individual variation.
02:42 - Recognizing Unsymbolized Thinking
Discussion about how people often don't realize they experience unsymbolized thinking until questioned about it.
03:19 - Core Definition and Characteristics
Detailed explanation of what makes something unsymbolized thinking.
04:25 - Key Features of Unsymbolized Thoughts
Discussion of the explicitness, differentiation, and direct presentation of unsymbolized thoughts.
05:45 - The All-at-Once Quality
Exploring how unsymbolized thoughts appear immediately and completely, without steps.
06:14 - Propositional Nature
Explanation of how unsymbolized thoughts have structure similar to sentences despite lacking words.
06:49 - Effability - Putting Thoughts into Words
Discussion of how unsymbolized thoughts can later be expressed in specific words.
Index with TimestampsAgency, sense of, 11:18, 12:55
All-at-once quality, 05:32, 05:49
Beeper method, 01:37, 01:45
Buddhist perspectives, 17:17, 17:24, 18:05
Cable prices example, 16:15
Carruthers, 09:22
Cognitive phenomenology, 15:37
Consciousness, implications for, 15:24
Descriptive experience sampling (DES), 01:31, 01:36
Differentiation in thought, 05:03, 05:13
Dorothy example, 07:25, 07:41
Effability, 06:55, 07:00
Emptiness (sunyata), 18:31
Explicit thought content, 04:59, 05:02
Feinberg and Frith, 11:45
Hu, Fernia, 14:45
Hurlburt, 04:01, 13:51
Inner speech, 00:37, 03:59, 08:30, 09:19, 09:41, 15:02
Intention to speak, 12:15, 13:00, 13:11
Language, influence of, 16:49, 19:07, 19:27, 19:42
Meaning, prediction of, 10:36, 12:26, 12:33, 14:09
Mirror, mind as, 17:52
Motor imagery, 09:31, 09:37
Nama concept, 19:04, 19:07
Nature of Unsymbolized Thinking, The, 01:17, 01:24
Phonological stage, 08:58, 09:00
Propositional nature, 06:17, 06:30, 11:00
Pure meanings, thinking in, 14:45
Research findings, 02:04, 02:17, 02:37
Semantic level, 12:28
Simulation of speech, 09:37, 09:41
Symbols, absence of, 00:51, 05:39, 05:43
Thought experiment for listeners, 20:45
Thought ownership, 11:28, 13:00, 13:11
Unworded speech, 13:24, 13:30, 14:58
Unsymbolized thinking (UT), definition, 00:45, 00:49, 03:24, 03:33
Vicente and Martinez-Manrique, 01:20, 08:34
Wordless thoughts, 02:53, 06:12
Words, absence of, 00:51, 03:34, 03:39, 05:43, 10:57, 13:42, 17:00
Poll
Post-Episode Fact Check
Image (3000 x 3000 pixels)
Mind Map
Word Search