When Hawks Become Traffic Engineers or The Arrogance of Human-Centered Thinking
This single observation of one hawk represents something much larger and more troubling: we're living on a planet full of intelligence we don't recognize because it doesn't look like ours.
With every article and podcast episode, we provide comprehensive study materials: References, Executive Summary, Briefing Document, Quiz, Essay Questions, Glossary, Timeline, Cast, FAQ, Table of Contents, Index, 3k Image, and Fact Check.
Please support my work by becoming a free subscriber. (Your subscription helps me continue providing evidence-based analysis in a media landscape increasingly hostile to inconvenient truths.)
We've built a world of concrete and steel, convinced we're the only intelligent architects navigating its complexities. Then a hawk shows up at a New Jersey intersection and shatters that delusion in the most spectacular way possible.
This isn't another feel-good story about nature's resilience. This is about confronting an uncomfortable truth: we've been drastically underestimating the cognitive abilities of the creatures we've displaced, and what that says about our own blind spots might be more unsettling than we're prepared to admit.
The Intersection Where Everything Changed
Picture this: West Orange, New Jersey. Morning rush hour. Cars queuing at a red light, drivers scrolling their phones, completely oblivious to the fact that they're being studied. Not by traffic engineers or urban planners, but by a young Cooper's hawk who has figured out something most humans never notice about their own infrastructure.
Here's where it gets wild. This hawk discovered that when pedestrians push the crosswalk button, it doesn't just change the light timing—it creates a predictable 90-second window where cars line up in a specific pattern. More importantly, the hawk learned to use the audible pedestrian signal as a cue. Not for food. Not for territory. But as a predictor of optimal hunting conditions.
Think about the cognitive architecture required here. This bird had to:
Map an entire neighborhood in three dimensions
Understand cause-and-effect relationships in human-built systems
Recognize that an audio cue predicted a visual change in traffic patterns
Develop a multi-stage hunting strategy that exploited this knowledge
Execute with precision timing
This isn't instinct. This isn't trial and error. This is strategic thinking that would impress a military tactician.
The Intelligence We Refuse to Acknowledge
We live in a culture that worships human exceptionalism while simultaneously proving how unexceptional our thinking often is. We've built entire academic careers around the assumption that complex reasoning belongs exclusively to us, maybe some primates, and if we're feeling generous, dolphins and cephalopods.
Meanwhile, this hawk is out here reverse-engineering traffic engineering better than most city planners understand their own systems.
The research team calculated the probability of this behavior occurring by chance: 0.0053%. That's not luck. That's not conditioning. That's problem-solving at a level that should make us deeply uncomfortable about our assumptions.
But here's what really gets me: this probably isn't even the hawk's most impressive cognitive feat. This is just the one we happened to notice because it intersected with our world in a way we could measure and quantify.
What We're Missing in the Woods
If a hawk can decode urban traffic patterns this quickly—and remember, many urban hawks are recent arrivals from rural areas—what kind of environmental intelligence are they demonstrating in their natural habitats that we're completely blind to?
We've spent centuries studying wildlife behavior while fundamentally misunderstanding what we're looking at. We've categorized, classified, and reduced animal behavior to simple inputs and outputs, completely missing the sophisticated decision-making processes happening right in front of us.
This hawk story forces an uncomfortable question: if we can't even recognize intelligence when it's literally using our own infrastructure against us, how much are we missing about the cognitive complexity of the natural world?
The Arrogance of Human-Centered Thinking
There's something deeply humbling about realizing that while we're stuck in traffic, frustrated by delays and inefficiencies in systems we designed, other species are studying those same systems and finding ways to exploit them for their own purposes.
We build cities thinking we're creating human-exclusive spaces, then act surprised when other creatures not only adapt but potentially understand our urban systems better than we do. This hawk didn't just adapt to city life—it hacked it.
And it's not alone. The research mentions other urban wildlife innovations: crows using cars to crack nuts, birds nesting in trains, raptors using streetlights for hunting. Each of these behaviors represents a cognitive leap that we've barely begun to understand.
The Bigger Picture We're Missing
This single observation of one hawk at one intersection represents something much larger and more troubling: we're living on a planet full of intelligence we don't recognize because it doesn't look like our own.
We've created a definition of intelligence so narrow and anthropocentric that we miss the sophisticated problem-solving happening all around us. We mistake different for inferior, and in doing so, we reveal more about our own cognitive limitations than theirs.
The hawk at that New Jersey intersection wasn't performing for researchers. It was solving a problem using available resources and novel information sources. It was being intelligent in a way that served its purposes, not ours.
What This Means for Everything Else
If we're this wrong about hawks—creatures we can observe directly—what does that say about our understanding of other species? What sophisticated behaviors and cognitive abilities are we missing because they don't fit our narrow definitions of intelligence?
More importantly, what does this mean for how we design our world? If we're sharing urban spaces with creatures whose intelligence we consistently underestimate, how should that change our approach to city planning, conservation, and coexistence?
This hawk story isn't just about animal cognition. It's about the dangerous arrogance of assuming we're the only ones paying attention, the only ones learning, the only ones capable of strategic thinking about the world we've built.
The Uncomfortable Truth
The most unsettling part of this story isn't that a hawk figured out traffic patterns. It's that it took us this long to notice, and even now, we're probably still missing most of what's actually happening.
We're living in a world full of intelligence we can't see because we're too busy being impressed with our own. Meanwhile, hawks are becoming traffic engineers, and we're just now catching up to what they figured out on their own.
Maybe it's time to start paying attention to who else is watching us, learning from us, and possibly laughing at how long it's taking us to realize we're not the only ones in the room with something interesting to say.
The hawk at that intersection didn't just hunt successfully. It held up a mirror to our assumptions about intelligence, adaptation, and who's really in control of the urban environment we think we designed.
And honestly? We don't look as smart as we thought we did.
References:
Street smarts: a remarkable adaptation in a city-wintering raptor
Find us:
YouTube
Substack
Podcast Providers
Spotify
Apple Podcasts
Patreon
FaceBook Group
STUDY MATERIALS
1. Briefing Document
Key Findings:
This case study details a unique observation of an immature Cooper's hawk (Accipiter cooperi) in West Orange, New Jersey, that developed a novel hunting strategy exploiting urban traffic patterns. The hawk was observed using queues of cars waiting at a red light as cover to approach prey and utilizing the sound signals for visually impaired pedestrians as an indicator of a longer red light cycle, and therefore, a longer window of opportunity with vehicular cover.
Main Themes and Important Ideas:
Urban Adaptation in Raptors: The study highlights the remarkable ability of urban birds, particularly raptors, to adapt to complex and challenging human-modified environments. Urban birds not only avoid dangers like vehicles but also learn to exploit them. The source provides examples of crows using cars to break nuts and other prey, carrion-eating birds monitoring roads for roadkill, passerines gleaning insects from cars, and small birds using cars as mobile shelters.
Cognitive Abilities of Accipiter Hawks: Accipiter hawks, known for their stealthy hunting techniques, require significant cognitive abilities to be successful. This includes planning ahead, understanding prey behavior and movement patterns, and being highly observant. The paper notes that their intelligence and flexibility contribute to their success in both natural and urban environments, with some species even showing improved hunting success in cities.
Novel Urban Hunting Techniques: Urban environments can lead to the development of novel hunting techniques not observed in rural populations. The study references examples of urban Eurasian goshawks using exposed vantage points, "backyard jumping" for surprise attacks, and hunting under artificial light. Other examples include Eurasian sparrowhawks chasing prey into confined spaces or large windows, and using moving vehicles for cover.
Cooper's Hawk Urban Colonization: Cooper's hawks have successfully colonized cities as breeding and wintering habitats since the 1970s, becoming the most successful urban colonizer among Accipiter hawks, despite being a medium-sized, hypercarnivorous raptor which goes against the general trend of smaller, generalist raptors being more successful in urban areas.
The Specific Observed Behavior: The core finding of the study is the detailed description of the immature Cooper's hawk's hunting strategy. The hawk:
Used queues of cars at a specific intersection as cover.
Appeared in a tree in front of house #11 when sound signals at the streetlight indicated a longer red light phase.
Used the longer car queue (extending up to house #8) to approach the prey flock feeding in front of house #2 undetected.
Flew at a low height (~1m) along the sidewalk and made a 90° turn between cars to reach the prey.
The sound signals were a statistically significant predictor of the hawk's appearance in the tree (P ≈ 0.000053).
Intellectual Feat: The observed behavior is described as a "remarkable intellectual feat for a young bird." It required the hawk to develop a "precise mental map of the street," understand the "connection between the sound signals and the length of the car queue," and recognize that "only longer queues provided cover for the entire approach."
Pre-existing Intelligence vs. Urban Evolution: The author suggests that the observed intelligence was likely "pre-existing rather than evolved in the novel environment." This is supported by observations of other species, like red foxes and urban chimango caracaras, which show behavioral changes but not necessarily increased intelligence in urban settings. The fact that many urban-wintering Cooper's hawks are likely recent immigrants from non-urban areas further supports this.
Limitations of a Case Study: The author acknowledges that this is a case study and difficult to replicate. However, they argue it fits within the broader context of complex hunting behaviors observed in raptors, such as cooperative hunting, tool use, and intentional fire-spreading.
Comparison to Pavlovian Training: While seemingly simple, the observed behavior is considered more complex than basic Pavlovian training, requiring a deeper understanding of the environment and its dynamics.
Future Observations and Change in Behavior: The observed behavior ceased when the sound signals stopped working and the food source (leftovers) was no longer available, highlighting the learned and opportunistic nature of the strategy. The likely same individual was observed using the same method as an adult in the following winter.
Key Quotes:
"In the present study, a particularly remarkable behavioral adaptation by Cooper’s hawk (Accipiter cooperi) was observed."
"The hawk apparently learned to prepare for attack when sound signals indicated the activation of pedestrian crossing regime."
"The observed behavior required having a mental map of the area and understanding the connection between the sound signals and the change in traffic pattern – a remarkable intellectual feat for a young bird that likely had just moved into the city."
"Such level of understanding and use of human traffic patterns by a wild animal has never been reported before."
"Urban hawks develop novel ways of hunting not observed in their 'rural' conspecifics."
"This appears to be the most advanced case of raptors adapting to use traffic patterns reported to date."
"To hunt as observed, the hawk had to develop a precise mental map of the street... notice the connection between the sound signals and the length of the car queue, and figure out that only longer queues provided cover for the entire approach."
"The behavior described here is an impressive feat of intelligence, going a long way to explain the species’ ability to successfully colonize such unusual and dangerous environment as urban landscape."
"This intelligence was likely pre-existing rather than evolved in the novel environment."
Conclusion:
The observation documented in this paper provides compelling evidence of a sophisticated behavioral adaptation in an urban Cooper's hawk, demonstrating a remarkable level of cognitive ability in a young raptor. The hawk's learned strategy of utilizing urban traffic patterns and sound signals for hunting highlights the intelligence and flexibility of these birds and sheds light on their success in urban environments. This study represents a novel finding in the field of urban wildlife adaptation and the cognitive abilities of birds of prey.
Further Actions:
Consider the implications of these findings for urban wildlife management and conservation.
Explore potential avenues for further research into the cognitive abilities of urban raptors and their adaptations to human-modified landscapes.
2. Quiz & Answer Key
Quiz
What remarkable behavioral adaptation by a Cooper's hawk was observed in this study?
Where and in what type of habitat were the observations for this study conducted?
What specific hunting strategy did the observed Cooper's hawk employ using cars?
What environmental cue did the hawk appear to use to anticipate longer car queues?
How did the duration of the red light phase at the intersection change when the pedestrian crossing regime was activated?
Approximately how long did the sound signals for the pedestrian crossing last?
What was the prey flock that the hawk was observed hunting primarily composed of?
How did the hawk ensure it remained unseen by its prey during its approach?
Besides using cars as cover, what other intellectual feats were suggested as necessary for the hawk's observed behavior?
What happened to the bird flock after each observed attack attempt?
Answer Key
The study observed a Cooper's hawk using queues of cars as cover to hunt birds near a road intersection.
The observations were conducted visually in urban habitat in West Orange, New Jersey.
The hawk used lines of cars waiting at a red light as cover to approach a flock of birds feeding in front of a nearby house.
The hawk apparently learned to prepare for attack when sound signals indicated the activation of the pedestrian crossing regime, which resulted in longer car queues.
The red light phase increased from 30 seconds to 90 seconds when a pedestrian pressed the crossing button.
The sound signals for the pedestrian crossing continued for 45 seconds.
The prey flock was primarily composed of house sparrows, mourning doves, and sometimes European starlings.
The hawk used the queues of cars as cover, making itself not visible to the potential prey until the final stage of the attack.
The observed behavior required the hawk to have a precise mental map of the area and understand the connection between the sound signals and the change in traffic pattern.
After each observed attack attempt, the bird flock scattered and never returned on the same morning.
3. Essay Questions
Discuss the significance of the observed Cooper's hawk behavior in the context of urban adaptation in raptors. How does this finding contribute to our understanding of animal intelligence and flexibility?
Analyze the methodology of the study. What were the strengths and limitations of conducting purely observational research in an urban environment?
Compare and contrast the novel hunting technique observed in the Cooper's hawk with other reported urban adaptations in Accipiter species, such as those listed for Eurasian goshawks and sparrowhawks.
Evaluate the potential evolutionary and cognitive implications of the observed behavior. To what extent does this study suggest pre-existing intelligence versus adaptation to a novel urban environment?
Consider the role of human activity, specifically traffic patterns and pedestrian signals, in creating the conditions that enabled this remarkable hunting behavior. How does this highlight the complex interactions between wildlife and urban environments?
4. Glossary of Key Terms
Accipiter cooperi: The scientific name for the Cooper's hawk, the raptor species that is the focus of the study.
Behavioral adaptation: A change in an animal's behavior that helps it survive and reproduce in its environment.
Urban habitat: An environment characterized by human settlements, buildings, roads, and infrastructure.
Winter residents: Animals that live in a particular area during the winter months but are not necessarily present year-round.
Queues: Lines of vehicles waiting, typically for a traffic light or to proceed.
Pedestrian crossing regime: The sequence of traffic signals and associated cues (like sound signals) that allow pedestrians to safely cross a street.
Sound signals: Auditory cues, often beeping sounds, activated at traffic lights to assist visually impaired pedestrians.
Mental map: A cognitive representation of a physical space, allowing an animal to navigate and understand spatial relationships.
Cognitive abilities: The mental processes involved in learning, memory, problem-solving, and decision-making.
Raptor: A bird of prey, characterized by keen eyesight, hooked beaks, and sharp talons for hunting.
Hypercarnivorous: Primarily or exclusively consuming meat.
Neophobia: The fear of new things or experiences.
Conspecifics: Individuals of the same species.
Case study: An in-depth investigation of a single individual, group, or phenomenon.
Pavlovian training: A type of learning in which an animal associates a neutral stimulus with a meaningful stimulus, leading to a learned response.
5. Timeline of Main Events
1970s: Cooper's hawks begin to utilize cities as breeding and, more commonly, wintering habitat.
Prior to 2021:Urban Eurasian sparrowhawks in Kharkiv, Ukraine are known to use moving cars and streetcars as cover to hunt prey.
Urban Eurasian goshawks in Kharkiv, Ukraine are also recently observed using moving cars as cover for hunting (Dmitry Strelkov, pers. comm.).
An adult Cooper's hawk is twice accidentally observed using the described hunting method at the study location.
Over the preceding ten years, only three summer records of Cooper's hawks in West Orange, New Jersey are noted on eBird.
November 2021: While driving through the intersection of Main Street and Side Street in West Orange, New Jersey, the author first notices an immature Cooper's hawk using queued cars as cover to approach birds feeding in front of house #2. This observation prompts the author to conduct further systematic observations.
December 5, 2021 - March 3, 2022: Systematic observations are conducted by the author from a parked car on Side Street. Observations occur on weekdays between 07:30 and 09:00 EST, for 30-60 minutes per day, totaling 12 hours over 18 days. Observations are limited to mornings without precipitation and when the bird flock is present.
During the 12 hours of observation (Dec 2021 - Mar 2022):Six attempted attacks by the immature Cooper's hawk are observed.
The entire course of the attacks is determined after the first two observations. The hawk consistently flies to a tree in front of house #11, perches while cars queue, takes off and flies low along the sidewalk, makes a 90-degree turn, and crosses Side Street between cars towards the prey flock.
In one observed attack, the hawk is seen flying away with a house sparrow.
In another observed attack, the hawk is observed eating a mourning dove on the ground.
After each attack attempt, the bird flock scatters and does not return the same morning.
During the fourth observed attack, the author notices the hawk appearing in the tree at house #11 when the pedestrian crossing sound signals are active.
In the two subsequent observed attacks, the hawk also appears in the tree when the sound signals are on, but before the long car queue forms. This connection between the sound signal and the hawk's appearance is determined to be non-random.
The hawk is never seen on weekends during this period.
Winter 2022–2023: The author twice accidentally observes an adult Cooper's hawk using the same hunting method at the same location. While likely the same individual as the immature hawk from the previous year, these observations are not included in the formal analysis due to a lack of recorded observation time and certainty of the bird's identity.
Summer 2023: The sound signals at the streetlight stop working for unknown reasons, and the residents of house #2 stop leaving leftovers.
After Summer 2023: No hawks are observed at the intersection.
December 3, 2024: The study manuscript is received.
April 7, 2025: The study manuscript is accepted.
May 23, 2025: The study article is published in Frontiers in Ethology.
Cast of Characters:
Vladimir Dinets (VD): The author of the study. He conducted the observations, collected and analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript. He is the corresponding author.
Immature Cooper's Hawk (presumably the same individual): The primary subject of the systematic observations conducted between December 2021 and March 2022. This hawk is observed utilizing queued cars and pedestrian sound signals to hunt prey in an urban environment.
Adult Cooper's Hawk (potentially the same individual): Twice observed using the same hunting method at the study location in the winter of 2022-2023.
Residents of House #2: Individuals who regularly ate their evening meals in front of their house, leaving leftovers that attracted the flock of birds. Their actions indirectly created the hunting opportunity for the hawk.
Kim Anderson: Mentioned as a personal communication regarding crows dropping food items onto roads.
Daan Drukker: Mentioned as a personal communication regarding small birds using moving cars as shelters from raptors.
Hadija Mustafa: Mentioned as a personal communication regarding small birds using moving cars as shelters from raptors.
Theo Squires: Mentioned as a personal communication regarding urban Eurasian sparrowhawks chasing prey into large glass windows.
Dmitry Strelkov: Mentioned as a personal communication regarding a Eurasian goshawk using moving cars as cover for hunting in Kharkiv, Ukraine.
Klaudia Modlinska, Gonzalo Ángel Ramírez-Cruz, Brandon Mak, Francisco Guil: Reviewers of the published article.
6. FAQ
What remarkable adaptation was observed in the Cooper's hawk in the study?
The study observed an immature Cooper's hawk in urban New Jersey using queues of cars waiting at a traffic light as cover to approach and hunt birds. What was particularly remarkable was the hawk's apparent ability to understand and utilize human traffic patterns by associating the sound signals for pedestrian crossings with longer red light phases and thus longer car queues, which provided sufficient concealment for its hunting strategy.
How did the hawk know when the car queues would be long enough for its hunting method?
The hawk learned to recognize the sound signals emitted by the pedestrian crossing system at the traffic light. These signals indicated a longer red light phase was activated. The hawk associated this sound with the formation of longer queues of cars, which provided the necessary visual cover for it to approach its prey undetected. The hawk would appear in a nearby tree as soon as the sound signals were activated.
Is this type of adaptation to human traffic patterns common in urban raptors?
While other urban birds have been documented exploiting traffic in various ways, such as crows using cars to crack nuts or birds gleaning insects from vehicles, the level of understanding and strategic use of dynamic human traffic patterns by a wild raptor for hunting, as observed in this Cooper's hawk, has never been reported before.
What does this behavior suggest about the intelligence of Cooper's hawks?
The observed behavior suggests a significant level of intelligence and cognitive ability in the Cooper's hawk. It required the bird to develop a mental map of the area, understand the causal relationship between the sound signals and the traffic light cycle, and plan its attack strategy based on the length of the car queue. This demonstrates sophisticated problem-solving skills and environmental awareness.
Is the intelligence observed in this hawk likely a new development due to urbanization?
The study suggests that the observed intelligence in the Cooper's hawk is likely a pre-existing trait rather than something that evolved specifically in response to the urban environment. The hawk observed was immature and likely a recent arrival to the city, indicating that this capacity for complex behavior is inherent to the species, which helps explain their success in colonizing urban areas.
What makes Cooper's hawks successful urban colonizers despite being hypercarnivorous raptors?
Cooper's hawks are medium-sized, specialized hunters, which is an exception to the general trend of smaller, more generalist raptors being more successful in urban environments. Their success is attributed to their intelligence and flexibility, which allows them to adapt their hunting techniques to the complex urban landscape, including developing novel methods not seen in their rural counterparts.
Were there other instances of this behavior observed?
Yes, in the winter following the primary observation period, an adult Cooper's hawk was accidentally observed twice using the same hunting method at the same location. It is highly likely this was the same individual that was immature the previous year. These observations were not included in the systematic analysis but support the idea that the behavior was successful and potentially retained.
What happened to the hawk and the observed hunting behavior after the study period?
After the study period, the sound signals at the streetlight stopped working, removing the key cue the hawk was using to predict traffic patterns. Additionally, the residents who were leaving food leftovers that attracted the prey birds stopped doing so. As a result, no hawks were observed using this specific hunting method at that intersection afterward.
7. Table of Contents
Introduction and Context 0:00 - Setting the Stage Welcome to Heliox deep dive into unexpected urban wildlife intelligence
1:15 - The Research Foundation Overview of the remarkable study and its jaw-dropping findings
Urban Wildlife Adaptations 2:30 - City Survival Challenges The difficult landscape cities present to wildlife
3:45 - Known Adaptive Behaviors Classic examples: crows cracking nuts, scavenger birds, nesting adaptations
5:00 - Small Bird Innovations Using cars as cover, gleaning insects, vehicular interactions
Excipitor Hawks: The Natural Hunters 6:15 - Species Introduction Cooper's hawks, Goshawks, Sparrow hawks and their hunting nature
7:30 - Baseline Intelligence Natural cognitive abilities: planning, prediction, observation skills
8:45 - Urban Success Stories Why moving to cities can boost hunting success for some raptors
Previously Known Urban Hunting Techniques 10:00 - Established Methods Rooftop perching, backyard jumping, artificial light foraging
11:30 - Dramatic Hunting Strategies Using alleyways, glass windows, and moving vehicles as hunting tools
13:00 - Cooper's Hawk Urban History Their successful city colonization since the 1970s
The Groundbreaking Discovery 14:15 - Location and Setting West Orange, New Jersey intersection: Main Street and Side Street
15:30 - Traffic Light Mechanics Normal 30-second cycles vs. 90-second pedestrian-activated cycles
17:00 - The Sound Signal Connection Audible pedestrian signals and their rare 3.75% activation rate
The Prey and Hunting Ground 18:45 - Target Species House sparrows, mourning doves, European starlings at house #2
19:30 - Food Source Reliability Resident feeding patterns creating predictable prey location
The Hawk's Strategic Method 20:15 - Initial Observations Documenting the immature Cooper's hawk's timed hunting attempts
21:30 - The Precise Sequence Perching at house #11, waiting for audible signals
23:00 - Traffic Queue Strategy Using growing car lines as dynamic cover
24:45 - The Attack Pattern Low flight, 65-meter approach, 90-degree turn, crossing between cars
Statistical Analysis and Significance 26:30 - Ruling Out Coincidence P-value of 0.000053 - statistical proof of intentional behavior
28:00 - Beyond Simple Conditioning Why this transcends basic Pavlovian responses
Cognitive Implications 29:15 - Required Mental Abilities Spatial mapping, cause-effect understanding, route planning
31:00 - Abstract Thinking Connecting auditory cues to visual environmental changes
32:45 - Comparison to Other Intelligent Species Corvids, mammals, and spatial problem-solving capabilities
Evolutionary vs. Adaptive Intelligence 34:30 - Pre-existing vs. Developed Abilities Evidence suggesting inherent rather than evolved urban intelligence
36:00 - Supporting Examples Urban foxes, caracaras, and learning ability studies
37:30 - Migration Patterns Young hawks dispersing from non-urban breeding areas
Broader Implications 39:00 - Natural Environment Intelligence What hawks might understand in wild habitats we haven't discovered
40:45 - Hidden Cognitive Abilities Wildlife intelligence operating "under our noses"
Conclusion and Reflection 42:00 - Key Takeaways Unprecedented understanding and strategic use of human systems
43:30 - Final Thoughts Animals watching, learning, and leveraging our world in unseen ways
45:00 - Recurring Themes Boundary dissolution, adaptive complexity, embodied knowledge, quantum uncertainty
46:00 - Closing and Resources Podcast exploration encouragement and Substack information
8. Index
Accipitor hawks - 6:15
Artificial lights - 10:00, 11:30
Attack pattern - 24:45
Audible signals - 15:30, 17:00, 21:30
Backyard jumping - 10:00
Behavioral shifts - 3:45
Birds of prey - 1:15, 6:15
Car queues - 15:30, 17:00, 23:00
Caracaras - 36:00
Carrion birds - 3:45
Cause and effect - 31:00, 42:00
City colonization - 13:00
Cognitive abilities - 7:30, 32:45
Cooper's hawk - 6:15, 13:00, 20:15
Corvids - 32:45
Crosswalk button - 15:30
Crows - 3:45, 32:45
European starlings - 18:45
Excipitor hawks - 6:15
Goshawks - 6:15, 10:00, 11:30
House number patterns - 15:30, 18:45, 21:30, 24:45
House sparrows - 18:45
Hunting success - 8:45
Intelligence baseline - 7:30
Main Street - 14:15
Mental mapping - 29:15
Morning rush hour - 14:15
Mourning doves - 18:45
Moving vehicles - 5:00, 11:30
Nesting adaptations - 5:00
New Jersey - 14:15
Opportunism - 3:45
Pavlovian conditioning - 28:00
Pedestrian signals - 15:30, 17:00
Perching behavior - 10:00, 21:30
Planning abilities - 7:30, 29:15
Prey location - 18:45, 19:30
P-value - 26:30
Red light cycles - 15:30
Roadkill - 3:45
Route planning - 32:45
Side Street - 14:15
Sparrow hawks - 6:15, 11:30
Spatial problem solving - 32:45
Statistical analysis - 26:30
Streetlights - 11:30
Traffic patterns - 1:15, 14:15, 23:00
Ukraine observations - 11:30
Urban adaptations - 2:30, 3:45
Urban foxes - 36:00
Urban success - 8:45, 13:00
Vehicle traffic - 2:30
Visually impaired pedestrians - 17:00
West Orange - 14:15
Wildlife flexibility - 3:45
9. Post-Episode Fact Check
✓ CONFIRMED FACTS:
The research was conducted by Dr. Vladimir Dinets, a zoologist at the University of Tennessee who studies animal behavior, ecology, and conservation
The observations took place in West Orange, New Jersey, during the winter of 2021-2022, specifically starting in November 2021
The hawk was a juvenile/immature Cooper's hawk that used pedestrian crossing signals as cues for hunting
Observations were conducted between 07:30 and 09:00 EST, for 30-60 minutes per day, totaling 12 hours over 18 days between December 5, 2021 and March 3, 2022
The hawk waited for the pedestrian signal sound which triggered longer red lights and car queues that provided hunting cover
The research was published in the journal Frontiers in Ethology in 2025
This represents "the most advanced case of raptors adapting to use traffic patterns reported to date" according to the research
TECHNICAL DETAILS: The episode accurately describes the complex hunting strategy, the statistical significance of the behavior, and the cognitive implications. The specific details about traffic light timing, car queue lengths, house numbers, and the hawk's flight pattern align with the published research findings.
MINOR DISCREPANCY: One source mentions Tennessee instead of New Jersey, but this appears to be an error in that single source, as all other sources consistently confirm West Orange, New Jersey as the location.
CONCLUSION: The episode content is scientifically accurate and based on legitimate peer-reviewed research published in a reputable journal.